Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 34
Filter
1.
Psicol. Estud. (Online) ; 27: e49027, 2022.
Article in Portuguese | LILACS, Index Psychology - journals | ID: biblio-1376061

ABSTRACT

RESUMO. Este artigo visa discutir a produção da personagem 'envolvido' pela narrativa midiática e seus efeitos na legitimação da necropolítica brasileira. Para tanto, faz uso das contribuições teóricas de Cecília Coimbra, Achille Mbembe e Judith Butler para analisar as repercussões das fake News nas redes sociais direcionadas ao jovem Marcos Vinícius da Silva, morto na favela da Maré, no Rio de Janeiro, quando ia para a escola. A maneira como esse assassinato foi tratado nas redes sociais oferece elementos para a compreensão da produção e ratificação da personagem 'envolvido' na justificação de mortes de pessoas que participaram (ou não) de situações ilícitas, colocando-os como sujeitos matáveis na atual política de gestão da morte. As considerações finais do trabalho assinalam a urgência de uma postura crítica por parte da mídia e um engajamento teórico-prático que desnaturalize a necropolítica e as relações de poder cada vez mais recrudescidas pela atual conjuntura política no Brasil.


RESUMEN. Este artículo analiza la producción del personaje 'involucrado' en la narrativa de los medios y sus efectos sobre la legitimación de la necropolítica brasileña. Para ello, utilizo las contribuciones teóricas de Cecilia Coimbra, Achille Mbembe y Judith Butler para analizar las repercusiones de las noticias falsas en las redes sociales dirigidas al joven Marcos Vinícius da Silva, quien murió en la favela Maré en Río de Janeiro mientras se dirigía a la escuela. La forma en que se trató este asesinato en las redes sociales, ofrece elementos para comprender la producción y la ratificación del personaje 'involucrado' en la justificación de la muerte de personas que participaron (o no) en situaciones ilícitas, ubicándolas como sujetos matables en la política actual de manejo de la muerte. Las consideraciones finales del trabajo apuntan a la urgencia de una actitud crítica por parte de los medios y un compromiso teórico-práctico que desnaturalice las relaciones cada vez más necropolíticas y de poder en Brasil.


ABSTRACT. This article discusses the production of the character 'involved' by the media narrative and its effects on the legitimation of the Brazilian necropolitics. To this end, we used the theoretical contributions by Cecília Coimbra, Achille Mbembe and Judith Butler to analyze the repercussions of fake news on social media directed against the young boy Marcos Vinícius da Silva, who was killed in the favela of Maré in Rio de Janeiro while on his way to school. The way this murder was addressed on social networks offers elements for understanding the production and ratification of the character 'involved' in justifying the deaths of people who participated or not in illicit situations, placing them as killable subjects in the current death management policy. The final considerations of the work point to the urgency of a critical approach by the media and a theoretical-practical engagement that denaturalizes necropolitics and the power relations increasingly intensified by the current political situation in Brazil.


Subject(s)
Humans , Politics , Bereavement , Social Networking , Disinformation , Prejudice , Psychology, Social/ethics , Power, Psychological , Police/ethics , Social Media/supply & distribution , Racism/psychology , Homicide/psychology
2.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 115(45): 11435-11441, 2018 11 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30397119

ABSTRACT

This commentary focuses on two important contrasts in the behavioral sciences: (i) default versus nondefault study populations, where default samples have been used disproportionately (for psychology, the default is undergraduates at major research universities), and (ii) the adoption of a distant versus close (engaged) attitude toward study samples. Previous research has shown a strong correlation between these contrasts, where default samples and distant perspectives are the norm. Distancing is sometimes seen as necessary for objectivity, and an engaged orientation is sometimes criticized as biased, advocacy research, especially if the researcher shares a social group membership with the study population (e.g., a black male researcher studying black male students). The lack of diversity in study samples has been paralleled by a lack of diversity in the researchers themselves. The salience of default samples and distancing in prior research creates potential (and presumed) risk factors for engaged research with nondefault samples. However, a distant perspective poses risks as well, and particularly so for research with nondefault populations. We suggest that engaged research can usefully encourage attention to the study context and taking the perspective of study samples, both of which are good research practices. More broadly, we argue that social and educational sciences need skepticism, interestedness, and engagement, not distancing. Fostering an engaged perspective in research may also foster a more diverse population of social scientists.


Subject(s)
Cognition , Cultural Diversity , Psychology, Social/methods , Research Design/trends , Humans , Individuality , Minority Groups/psychology , Psychology, Social/ethics , Race Factors , Race Relations/psychology , Risk Factors
3.
Am J Community Psychol ; 62(1-2): 221-232, 2018 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30216460

ABSTRACT

The focus of this paper is to demonstrate how embodied subjectivities shape research experiences. Through an autoethnography of my involvement in a Youth Participatory Action Research (YPAR) after-school program with low-income and working-class youth of Color from predominantly Latinx communities I examined my embodied subjectivities, via an ethical reflective practice, as these surfaced in the research context. Autoethnography is presented as a tool to facilitate an ethical reflective practice that aligns with heart-centered work. Drawing from an epistemology of a theory in the flesh (Anzaldúa & Moraga, 1981), embodied subjectivities are defined by the lived experiences felt and expressed through the body, identities, and positionalities of the researcher. The article concludes with implications for the development of community psychology competencies that attend to the researcher's embodied subjectivities.


Subject(s)
Anthropology, Cultural/ethics , Community-Based Participatory Research/ethics , Psychology, Social/ethics , Adolescent , Anthropology, Cultural/methods , Community-Based Participatory Research/methods , Humans , Psychology, Social/methods , Schools
5.
Rev. psicol. deport ; 24(1): 185-190, 2015. tab, ilus
Article in Spanish | IBECS | ID: ibc-132199

ABSTRACT

El presente estudio se ha realizado con el doble objetivo de ampliar el modelo de medida del liderazgo deportivo entre iguales (EELD-30) elaborado por Arce, Torrado, Andrade y Alzate (2011) y a la vez reducir el número de indicadores para mejorar su aplicabilidad. Como resultado, se ha obtenido una versión, denominada EELD-24, con más factores (seis en lugar de cinco) y con menor número de ítems (24 en lugar de 30). El ajuste individual de los parámetros (cargas factoriales de los ítems, correlaciones entre factores, y varianzas de los errores de medida) ha sido satisfactorio y los índices de ajuste global del modelo razonables: χ2 (242) = 566.278 (p < .001); χ2/df = 2.340; GFI = 0.90; TLI = 0.92; CFI = 0.93; SRMR = 0.058; RMSEA = 0.058 ( I.C. 90%; 0.052; 0.064). También se han obtenido resultados satisfactorios de consistencia interna de los factores, con valores de alpha de Cronbach que oscilan entre .76 y .86, de validez convergente y discriminante, y de invarianza del modelo de medida. La escala resultante puede ser utilizada con fines profesionales y de investigación con las suficientes garantías psicométricas


The present study was developed with the aim of increasing the assessment model of peer leadership (EELD-30) elaborated by Arce, Torrado, Andrade and Alzate (2011) and, at the same time, shortening the number of indicators in order to improve its applicability. As a result, a new version was obtained, called EELD-24, with one more factor (six instead of five) and fewer items (24 instead of 30). Fit indexes (item loads, factor correlations and error variance) were satisfactory and global fit indexes of the model were reasonable: χ2(242) = 566.278 (p < .001); χ2/df = 2.340; GFI = 0.90; TLI = 0.92; CFI = 0.93; SRMR = 0.058; RMSEA = 0.058 (C.I. 90%; 0.052; 0.064). Apart from this, satisfactory indexes of internal consistence were obtained, with values of Cronbach’s Alpha between 0.76 and 0.86, and also convergent and discriminant validity and invariance of the model offer satisfactory results. The final scale can be used both for an applied context and for research with enough psychometric guarantees


O presente estudo foi realizado com um objectivo duplo de ampliar o modelo de medida de liderança desportiva entre pares (EELD-30)elaborado por Arce, Torrado, Andrade e Alzate (2011) e de reduzir o número de indicadores de forma a melhorar a sua aplicabilidade. Como resultado,foi obtida uma versão, denominada EELD-24, com mais factores (seis em vez de cinco) e com menor número de itens (24 em vez de 30). O ajustamentoindividual dos parâmetros (cargas factoriais dos itens, correlações entre factores e variâncias dos erros de medida) foi satisfatório e os índices deajustamento global do modelo razoáveis: χ2(242)= 566.278 (p< .001); χ2/df= 2.340; GFI = 0.90; TLI = 0.92; CFI = 0.93; SRMR = 0.058; RMSEA =0.058 (I.C. 90%; 0.052; 0.064). De igual modo foram obtidos resultados satisfatórios de consistência interna dos factores, com valores de alfa de Cronbachque oscilam entre os .76 e .86, de validade convergente e discriminante, e de invariância do modelo de medida. A escala resultante pode ser utilizada emcontexto aplicado e de investigação com suficientes garantias psicométricas


Subject(s)
Humans , Male , Sports/education , Sports/ethics , Psychology, Social/ethics , Psychology, Social , Sports/psychology , Sports/standards , Psychology, Social/education , Psychology, Social/methods
10.
Br J Soc Psychol ; 53(1): 54-73, 2014 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23106687

ABSTRACT

This article deals with the topic of social psychological research methods in practice, by examining how informed consent is gained from research participants. In most research, the consent-gaining process is hidden from analytic scrutiny and is dealt with before data collection has begun. In contrast, conversation analytic research, which records interactional encounters from beginning to end, enables examination of this methodological 'black box'. We explored how 'requests' to consent in research played out across different institutional settings. We found that participants had to 'opt-out' of a research process that was already underway. Consent-gaining sequences constrained opting out in two ways: (1) because research activity was already underway, it must be stopped affirmatively by participants; (2) consent-gaining turns were tilted in favour of continued participation, making opting out a dispreferred response. We also found a mismatch between what ethics guidelines specify about consent-gaining 'in theory' and what actually happens 'in practice'. Finally, we make suggestions about interventions in and recommendations for existing practice to best achieve informed consent.


Subject(s)
Communication , Ethics, Research , Informed Consent/ethics , Psychology, Social/ethics , Humans , United Kingdom
11.
J Health Psychol ; 19(1): 126-36, 2014 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24155187

ABSTRACT

The article sets out the value of theorizing collective action from a social science perspective that engages with the messy actuality of practice. It argues that community health psychology relies on an abstract version of Paulo Freire's earlier writing, the Pedagogy of the Oppressed, which provides scholar-activists with a 'map' approach to collective action. The article revisits Freire's later work, the Pedagogy of Hope, and argues for the importance of developing a 'journey' approach to collective action. Theories of practice are discussed for their value in theorizing such journeys, and in bringing maps (intentions) and journeys (actuality) closer together.


Subject(s)
Behavioral Medicine/methods , Community Participation/methods , Psychological Theory , Psychology, Social/methods , Behavioral Medicine/ethics , Humans , Psychology, Social/ethics , Residence Characteristics
12.
Psicopedagogia ; 30(93): 2012-2017, 2013.
Article in Portuguese | Index Psychology - journals | ID: psi-62140

ABSTRACT

Sem negar a importância de se pensar na questão da inclusão e no provimento de ações capazes de atender às necessidades especiais dos indivíduos e grupos, o presente trabalho pretende refletir sobre o tema a partir do conceito de desamparo estrutural e do trabalho da cria humana para aceder à ordem da cultura. Tal consideração aponta para o perigo de medidas protetoras e paternalistas, que no lugar de orientarem-se em direção ao compromisso ético, reforçam a dependência, a submissão e a infantilização. Assim, o ponto vivo da reflexão do presente trabalho é trabalhar a ideia de que toda e qualquer ação de inclusão deve ter como horizonte a construção do rosto. Construção nascida dos embates entre desamparo estrutural e a Ananké orientará o homem em seu compromisso ético de inventar com trabalho e pena seu caminho na história humana.(AU)


Without denying the importance of considering the issue of inclusion and provision of actions capable of meeting the special needs of individuals and groups, this article aims to reflect on the topic from the perspective of the concept of structural helplessness and the effort of the human infant to get into culture. This consideration points to the danger of paternalistic and protective measures, that differently from an ethical commitment, reinforce dependency, submissiveness and infantilization. Thus the most important point of this article is to discuss the idea that any action of inclusion should aim at the development of a face. The outcome of clashes between structural helplessness and Ananke guide us in one ethical commitment to create through labour and sorrow our own way in human history.(AU)


Subject(s)
Mainstreaming, Education/ethics , Psychology, Social/ethics , Socialization
13.
Science ; 338(6112): 1270-1, 2012 Dec 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23224529
17.
An. psicol ; 27(3): 575-581, oct.-dic. 2011.
Article in English | IBECS | ID: ibc-94294

ABSTRACT

El objetivo de este Tema Monográfico ha sido reunir investigaciones de psicólogos evolutivos y sociales, españoles y extranjeros, en el amplio ámbito del prejuicio y las relaciones intergrupales. Se incluyen trabajos de revisión y estudios empíricos con distintos presupuestos teóricos, procedimientos de investigación y tipo de poblaciones. La mayoría de las contribuciones abordan el estudio de las actitudes de niños, adolescentes y jóvenes hacia la diversidad étnico-racial, el sesgo endogrupal o la identidad étnica, y tres artículos tratan otras formas de prejuicio intergrupal de innegable importancia social en nuestra cultura occidental: los prejuicios hacia la homosexualidad, la discapacidad y la gordura (AU)


The aim of this Special Issue is to gather research from developmental and social psychologists, from Spain and other countries, in the broad area of prejudice and intergroup attitudes. The Monograph includes review papers and empirical studies that present different theoretical assumptions, research procedures, and types of population. Most contributions are related to the study of children’s and youth’s attitudes toward different racial-ethnic groups, in group bias or ethnic identity, and three articles address other forms of prejudice that have an undeniable social significance in our Western societies: biases toward homosexuality, disability, and body size (AU)


Subject(s)
Humans , Male , Female , Child , Adolescent , Young Adult , Adult , Psychology , Psychology, Social/education , Psychology, Social/history , Prejudice , Ethnopsychology/education , Psychology, Social/ethics , Psychology, Social/organization & administration , Psychology, Social/trends , Ethnopsychology/statistics & numerical data , Ethnopsychology/trends
18.
An. psicol ; 27(3): 708-717, oct.-dic. 2011. tab
Article in English | IBECS | ID: ibc-94309

ABSTRACT

En esta investigación se ha evaluado un nuevo concepto en psicología social, denominado "contacto imaginado‟, en niños pequeños (n = 123, de 5 a 10 años). A la mitad de la muestra se les pidió que se imaginaran a sí mismos interactuando con un niño con una discapacidad física; la otra mitad no participó en esta actividad (grupo control). En comparación con el grupo control, los niños en la condición "contacto imaginado‟ mostraron posteriormente una reducción del sesgo intergrupal cuando se evaluaban sus actitudes generales y sus valoraciones de cordialidad y aptitudes. El "contacto imaginado‟ también se asoció con un mayor deseo de desarrollar conductas de amistad hacia el niño con discapacidad, pero solo en los participantes de 5 -6 años. Esto apoya parcialmente nuestra hipótesis de que los niños pequeños, quizá como resultado de una falta de experiencia con el exogrupo, se benefician en mayor medida del contacto imaginado. Se discuten las implicaciones para el desarrollo de las actitudes hacia las personas con discapacidad, la teoría del contacto imaginado y el desarrollo de técnicas de reducción de los prejuicios en el aula basadas en el contacto imaginado (AU)


The current research tested a recent development in social psychology, namely "imagined contact‟, among young children (n = 123, 5 to 10 years). Children imagined interacting with a physically disabled child, or did not take part in this activity (the control group). Compared with the control group, children who engaged in "imagined contact‟ subsequently showed reduced intergroup bias in their general attitude and ratings of warmth and competence. Imagined contact also led to more positive intended friendship behavior towards the disabled, but only among 5 – 6 year olds. This provides partial support for our hypothesis that younger children, perhaps as a result of their lack of out-group experience, are more likely to benefit from imagined contact. Implications for the development of attitudes towards the disabled, imagined contact theory and the development of classroom-based prejudice-reduction techniques based on imagined contact are discussed (AU)


Subject(s)
Humans , Male , Female , Child , Disabled Children/education , Disabled Children/psychology , Psychology, Social/ethics , Imagination/ethics , Disabled Children/rehabilitation , Disabled Children/statistics & numerical data , Psychology, Social/education , Psychology, Social/methods , Psychology, Social/statistics & numerical data , Imagination/classification , Imagination/physiology
20.
Sci Eng Ethics ; 15(1): 69-79, 2009 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18704760

ABSTRACT

All agree that if the Milgram experiments were proposed today they would never receive approval from a research ethics board. However, the results of the Milgram experiments are widely cited across a broad range of academic literature from psychology to moral philosophy. While interpretations of the experiments vary, few commentators, especially philosophers, have expressed doubts about the basic soundness of the results. What I argue in this paper is that this general approach to the experiments might be in error. I will show that the ethical problems that would prevent the experiments from being approved today actually have an effect on the results such that the experiments might show less than many currently suppose. Making this case demonstrates two conclusions. The first is that there are good reasons to think that the conclusions of many of Milgram's commentators might be too strong. The second conclusion is a more general one. The ethics procedures commonly used by North American research ethics boards serve not only to protect human participants in research but also can sometimes help secure, to an extent, the integrity of results. In other words, good ethics can sometimes mean better science.


Subject(s)
Behavioral Research/ethics , Human Experimentation/ethics , Psychology, Social/ethics , Ethics Committees, Research , Ethics, Research , Humans , Informed Consent/ethics , North America
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...